Monday, 8 June 2015

Jeff Borchardt-- I typed this into the google search engine..."how to train a pointer to point" and this is the result--About 1,410,000 results (0.44 seconds)- instincts mean nothing unless their nurtured promoted and TRAINED!!

How to Train a Pointer to Hunt

Read more : http://www.ehow.com/how_2154776_train-pointer-hunt.html

Pointer Puppyhood

  • While your pointer should enjoy his puppyhood as much as any nonsporting dog, avoid letting him get into bad habits. Cute puppy behavior -- such as refusing to "let go" during tug-of-war games -- isn't quite so adorable when a grown pointer refuses to give up a bird. Socialize him, so he gets used to other dogs, people and places. Introduce him to his future life as a field hunter in a fun, constructive way, without pressure. That includes lots of games of fetch in the backyard. Get him used to gunshots -- but not by suddenly shooting above his head. Use a cap gun initially, making low shots. If he's frightened, act as if nothing has happened and continue shooting periodically during play sessions, until your puppy shows no concern.

Basic Commands

  • All canines should learn basic commands, but for hunting dogs working in the field, it's a necessity for proper conditioning. Take your pointer to puppy kindergarten or basic obedience classes. Besides the ordinary commands, such as "come" and "sit and stay," a pointer must know "hunt" or "seek" and "fetch." Before your pointer is ready to start bird work, he must obey your sit/stay command. He'll also need to know "stop" or "whoa." Pointers must know how to stand absolutely still. While stopping to point is innate in many such dogs, he must learn to stay put until you let him know he can move again.

Training for Birds

  • Beginning true bird training means you must have access to sufficient land on which to hunt and fire shots. You must train before the start of hunting season, when birds are still plentiful on the ground. Sometimes, just letting your young pointer chase numerous birds until he's so tired he'll point instead of chase -- followed by effusive praise -- allows him to get the idea. Working alongside a seasoned pointer when your young animal begins to get the concept of pointing also helps to teach him what's expected. Your dog should start spontaneously pointing when he scents prey.

Training for Shooting

  • Once your pointer is unfazed by gunshots, you can start training for shooting. If possible, have a friend do the initial firing so you can watch your dog's reactions. Your dog must sit/stay while shooting commences. When a bird is downed, make your dog waits several seconds, then tell him to "fetch." Once he has the bird in his mouth, tell him to "come" and give him lots of praise when he does. As he improves, you start firing the gun. Give your dog time and lots of practice, and by the end of the season you should have a reliable field pointer.
Read more : http://www.ehow.com/how_2154776_train-pointer-hunt.html

100 Years of Breed “Improvement” by Caen Elegans

For the sake of honest disclosure, I will admit to owning “purebreds” (the ‘pureness’ of purebreeds is a discussion for another time) but I also have mutts. All the dogs I’ve had since childhood had a few things in common, they were friendly, prey driven, ball-crazy, intense, motivated, athletic (crazy dogs are easier to train) and none had intentionally bred defects. I would never buy/adopt a dog whose breed characteristics exacted a health burden.(Asher 2009). That just incentivizes people to breed more of these intentionally unhealthy animals. The dogs on the left are from  the 1915 book, ‘Breeds of All Nations by W.E. Mason. The examples on the right are modern examples from multiple sources. To be able to make an honest comparison, I’ve chosen pictures with similar poses and in a couple of cases flipped the picture to get them both aligned in the same direction. I had to skip some breeds I wanted to include because of the lack of detail in the older photographs.
It seems incredible that at one time the Bull Terrier was a handsome, athletic dog. Somewhere along its journey to a mutated skull and thick abdomen the bull terrier also picked up a number of other maladies like supernumerary teeth and compulsive tail-chasing.

The Basset Hound has gotten lower, has suffered changes to its rear leg structure, has excessive skin, vertebra problems, droopy eyes prone to entropion and ectropion and excessively large ears.

A shorter face means a host of problems. The modern Boxer not only has a shorter face but the muzzle is slightly upturned. The boxer – like all bracecyphalic dogs – has difficulty controlling its temperature in hot weather, the inability to shed heat places limits on physical performance. It also has one of the highest cancer rates.

The English bulldog has come to symbolize all that is wrong with the dog fancy and not without good reason; they suffer from almost every possible disease. A 2004 survey by the Kennel Club found that they die at the median age of 6.25 years (n=180). There really is no such thing as a healthy bulldog. The bulldog’s monstrous proportions make them virtually incapable of mating or birthing without medical intervention.

The Dachshund used to have functional legs and necks that made sense for their size. Backs and necks have gotten longer, chest jutted forward and legs have shrunk to such proportions that there is barely any clearance between the chest and floor. The dachschund has the highest risk of any breed for intervertebral disc disease which can result in paralysis; they are also prone to achondroplastic related pathologies, PRA and problems with their legs.

The German Shepherd Dog is also a breed that is routinely mentioned when people talk about ruined breeds; maybe because they used to be awesome. In Dogs of All Nations, the GSD is described as a medium-sized dog (25 kg /55 lb), this is a far cry from the angulated, barrel-chested, sloping back, ataxic, 85-pounders  (38 kg) we are used to seeing in the conformation ring. There was a time when the GSD could clear a 2.5 meter (8.5 ft) wall; that time is long gone.

The Pug is another extreme brachycephalic breed and it has all the problems associated with that trait – high blood pressure, heart problems, low oxygenation, difficulty breathing, tendency to overheat, dentition problems, and skin fold dermatitis. The highly desirable double-curl tail is actually a genetic defect, in more serious forms it leads to paralysis.

Once a noble working dog, the modern St. Bernard has been oversized, had its faced squished in, and bred for abundant skin. You will not see this type of dog working, they can’t handle it as they quickly overheat. The diseases include entropion, ectropion, Stockard’s paralysis, hemophilia, osteosarcoma, aphakia, fibrinogen deficiency.

It is unrealistic to expect any population to be free of genetic diseases but show breeders have intentionally selected for traits which result in diseases. Conformation breeders claim they are improving the breed and yet they are often the cause of these problems. If “improvement” in looks imposes a health burden then it is not a breed improvement..
No dog breed has ever been improved by the capricious and arbitrary decision that a shorter/longer/flatter/bigger/smaller/curlier “whatever” is better.  Condemning a dog to a lifetime of suffering for the sake of looks is not an improvement; it is torture.
Further Reading
REFERENCES
Asher L, Diesel G, Summers JF, McGreevy PD, Collins LM. (2009). Inherited defects in pedigree dogs. Part 1: disorders related to breed standards.  Vet J. 2009 Dec;182(3):402-11.


  1. Bull Terrier
  2. Basset Hound
  3. Boxer
  4. Bulldog
  5. German Shepherd Dog
  6. Pug
  7. Saint Bernard

What is the RSPCA's position on breed-specific legislation?

The RSPCA does not support breed specific legislation, also known as BSL. RSPCA Australia considers that any dog of any size, breed or mix of breeds may be dangerous and thus dogs should not be declared dangerous on the basis of breed or appearance. Each individual dog should be assessed based on their behaviour.






The RSPCA does not believe that BSL is in any way effective in preventing or reducing dog attacks or in protecting the public from dangerous dogs.



The RSPCA believes that a prevention strategy for dog attacks must contain the following key elements:

  • Registration and microchipping of all dogs: so that all dogs are traceable to their owners and that owners can be directly informed of their legal responsibilities.
  • Control of unrestrained and free-roaming animals: through the resourcing of local councils to enforce existing dog control provisions.
  • Provisions for the control of menacing dogs: measures are put in place to intervene early where a dog has exhibited repeated threatening behaviour but does not meet the definition of a dangerous dog.
  • Desexing of non-breeding dogs: male entire dogs are at greater risk of aggression and female entire dogs add to this risk by attracting entire males. Increased desexing rates can be achieved through desexing prior to sexual maturity, mandatory desexing prior to rehoming and breeder registration for entire dogs.
  • Education of the public, and particularly children, in dog behaviour and bite prevention.
  • Training of owners and dogs: training programs based on positive reinforcement techniques provide an opportunity to educate owners on responsible dog ownership, basic dog behaviour and the use of appropriate training techniques
  • Socialisation with people and other animals: unsocialised dogs are more likely to show aggressive behaviour; designated off-leash areas provide opportunities for safe socialisation.






For a full explanation of the RSPCA's position please read theInformation Paper - Preventing dog attacks in the community below.

More information is also available here: Australian Veterinary Association (2012) Dangerous dogs - a sensible solution.

This website provides general information which must not be relied upon or regarded as a substitute for specific professional advice, including veterinary advice. We make no warranties that the website is accurate or suitable for a person's unique circumstances and provide the website on the basis that all persons accessing the website responsibly assess the relevance and accuracy of its content.
Attached files
filePP A1 Control of dangerous and menacing dogs.pdf (158 kb)
fileRSPCA Information Paper - Preventing dog attacks in the community..pdf (349 kb)

Also read
documentIs there legislation relating to the ownership of specific breeds of dog?
documentHow can we help to prevent dog attacks in the community?
documentRSPCA Policy A08 Dog management

Dangerous dogs and dog bite prevention


Periodically the community is faced with horrific dog bite incidents, and sadly the victims are often children. These incidents tend to attract extensive media attention, accompanied by simplistic ‘solutions’ to the problem of dog aggression. These events are devastating for the families and naturally draw emotional responses from the community, putting politicians and governments under pressure to address the issue in a public way.
Policy responses to dog bites have increasingly turned to banning or controlling particular breeds of dogs (breed-specific legislation or BSL). Under pressure from the media, governments have established regulatory responses that give the community a false sense of security, allowing them to believe that they are safer from aggressive dogs. However,because these measures do not actually solve the underlying problems, similar dog bite incidents continue. 
Most dog bites take place in homes with familiar family pets, and most people bitten by dogs are children under 10 years of age. No bite prevention strategy can be successful without taking steps to reduce these incidents, numerically far greater than bites taking place in public places and caused by unknown dogs.
The Australian Veterinary Association (AVA) has released a comprehensive report, Dangerous dogs – a sensible solution which sets out the scientific evidence about dog bite incidents, and explores the factors that influence a dog’s tendency to bite. It demonstrates the ineffectiveness of breed-specific legislation in addressing aggressive behaviour in dogs, explaining that this approach does not increase public safety and is unworkable. 
The report highlights a scientific evidence-based policy alternative that combines recognition and control of ‘potentially dangerous dogs’ along with ‘dangerous dogs’. But regulation alone cannot reduce dog bite incidents. It must be combined with a strong system encouraging socially-responsible pet ownership, enforcing compliance with pet identification, registration and restraint rules, while educating the whole community including children about safety around dogs. The report also emphasises the importance of a national register of dog bite incidents and mandatory reporting of incidents to provide a solid evidence base for future policy decisions.
Dangerous dogs – a sensible solution - policy and model legislative framework August 2012

INQUIRY INTO THE EFFECTIVENESS OF BREED SPECIFIC LEGISLATION (BSL) IN QLD

TO: QUEENSLAND PARLIAMENT, DR ANTHONY LYNHAM MP FOR BRIS CENTRAL

INQUIRY INTO THE EFFECTIVENESS OF BREED SPECIFIC LEGISLATION (BSL) IN QLD
150 
of 200 signatures
Campaign created by Nigel GreenhalghIcon-email
Dear Honourable Ministers of the Queensland Parliament,
Please support a parliamentary inquiry into the effectiveness of breed specific legislation in keeping the Queensland public safe from dog attacks. This inquiry should include a review of how Queensland compares to international best practice in responsible dog ownership laws, conducted by a panel of appropriately qualified and experienced experts.
The Queensland community deserves to be kept safe from dog attacks by the best possible laws. The overwhelming evidence is that breed specific legislation is ineffective in preventing dog bites and worldwide breed specific laws are being revoked. The current laws should be reviewed for effectiveness and alternative regulatory models successful elsewhere, like the Calgary model, explored.
It is scientifically proven that many factors other than genetics can be attributed to dog bite incidents including early experience, health, socialisation and victim behaviour. As acknowledged by The Australian Veterinary Association in their report: Dangerous dogs – a sensible solution, most factors relate to the way that a dog is raised, kept and trained, managed. Of the key contributing factors to dog bite injuries identified by the AVA, 80% of these factors are not related to breed.
The Australian Veterinary Association and RSPCA do not support breed specific legislation as a means for protecting the community from dog attacks. Their policy positions and links to further information are provided below.
“Breed-specific legislation (BSL) for dog bite prevention is not supported as experience in other countries has shown that such legislation has failed to reduce the frequency of dog bites.” The Australian Veterinary Association, 

“The RSPCA does not support breed specific legislation, also known as BSL. Our view, based on the available international scientific evidence, is that any dog may be dangerous and that dogs should not be declared as ‘dangerous’ on the basis of breed. While we recognise that there is a strong genetic component in a dog’s propensity for aggressive behaviour, their trigger point for aggression and capacity to inflict serious injury, these factors are not isolated to any specific breed. The RSPCA does not believe that BSL is in any way effective in preventing or reducing dog attacks or in protecting the public from dangerous dogs.” 

Why is this important?

Authorised Officers of Local Governments have the authority to seize a suspected dangerous breed from it's owner and impound it at cost to the owner to conduct a 22-point visual test to determine if it's an American Pit Bull Terrier. The breed certificate nor any DNA tests can influence the decision of the council to decide if it's destroyed at the owner's expense. This can happen under the current legislation without the dog even being involved in any anti-social behaviours or attacks on other animals or humans prior to the test! Family dogs are being seized and put down without any proof of the breed (aside from a "visual" test lacking any scientific merit) or prior incidences that would deem it dangerous, and at the owner's expense!! 
It’s not the breed that makes a dog dangerous; it’s the irresponsible attitude of the owner. Indeed, it is the deed that should be punished, not the breed.

Study Says Human Behavior -- Not Dog Breed -- Predicts Canine Aggression

This post was created by a member of the Dodo Community, where anyone can post fascinating stories, photos, videos and more.
Different dog breeds have different temperaments, which is partly why dog-lovers are sometimes partial to one breed over others. But not all seemingly unique breed behaviors are actually breed-specific, specifically aggression. A recent study indicates that dog owners’ behavior -- not the dog’s breed -- could be key to predicting whether or not a dog will be aggressive.
The research could be one more nail in the coffin of breed-specific legislation, which unduly discriminates against certain breeds based on the assumption that they are inherently dangerous. According to the study, the type of training an owner uses plays a larger role in how their dog will interact with humans -- strangers and family members alike -- than the breed. Attending puppy classes was shown to reduce the risk of aggression to unfamiliar people, while training methods that used negative reinforcement increased the chance of aggression to, well, everyone. Additionally, dogs with owners over age 40 were two times less likely to have aggression issues than those with owners under age 25.
Ultimately, the researchers conclude that training dogs early -- preferably using positive reinforcement methods -- can decrease the likelihood that the dog will become aggressive, despite what rumors might suggest about the breed. While it might make sense to generalize about some canine behavioral tendencies, they say, it would be wrong to assume that an individual dog will be aggressive simply because of its breed. 

Does your dog's breed really determine its personality? laurendavis (Lauren Davis)

Does your dog's breed really determine its personality?1
Dog enthusiasts will frequently tell you that certain dogs have certain characteristics: Golden Retrievers are friendly, German Shepherds are protective, and Corgis are smarter than a fifth grader. But is a dog's breed really such a great predictor of its personality?
Does your dog's breed really determine its personality?
This is Skoda—or Dr. Emil Skoda if you want to be formal. He is, as far as I know, roughly seven years old and a purebred Boxer. He's going to help us out in this discussion.

What Defines a Breed, Anyway?

We can't really talk about whether breeds are predictive of personality without first understanding what a breed is. Breed, naturally, has to do with breeding. Skoda is (presumably) a Boxer because his dame and sire were (presumably) both Boxers. Simple, right?

Well, it's a little more complicated than that. Many of our current dog breeds are fairly recent innovations, a product of Victorian-era attempts to produce superior dogs. The early breeders responsible for our modern purebred dogs actually crossbred dogs with characteristics they found desirable, and eventually created a genetically limited pool of dogs that would reliably produce pups with similar traits. The Boxer, for example, goes back just 150 years, after breeders began crossing a progenitor of the English Bulldog with a now-extinct breed known as the Brabanter Bullenbeisser. Skoda is a Boxer because he is descended from dogs that someone has termed "Boxer," but he, like other Boxers, is fairly recently related to Bulldogs and any other dog bred from the Bullenbeisser. Today, dog breeds represent closed gene pools; in order to qualify as a Boxer, Skoda has to come from an unbroken lineage of Boxers—with no ancestors from any other breed—going back decades.

Most dog lovers recognize Skoda as a Boxer (although he's occasionally mistaken for a Bulldog or a Pit Bull) because of his smushed-in nose, his lean body, his short, floppy ears, his arched skull, and that adorable underbite. (The snaggletooth, while cute, is one of many reasons he doesn't quite physically conform to breed standards.) They don't check to see if he's suspicious of strangers (he is), patient with children (yes, but he hates babies), alert (to a fault), or energetic (he starts bouncing up and down at the mere mention of a W-A-L-K), but they do make assumptions about his personality.

This recognition of breeds through appearance more or less fits with the breed standards as defined by show organizations like Britain's Kennel Club and the American Kennel Club. If you watch conformation shows like Crufts or the Westminster Kennel Club Dog Show, the judges are looking for physical conformation to breed standards. If Skoda were to compete in a conformation show (something that wouldn't happen for a number of reasons), judges would be looking at his size, the shape of his body, the color of his coat, his gait, the proportions of his skull, and a host of other largely physical traits. (Although he would be penalized for "evidence of shyness, or lack of dignity or alertness," so my wary goofball might not do well on that front, either.)

Kennel club breed standards are controversial on a number of fronts, not the least of which is concern that certain physically desired traits aren't particularly healthy for dogs.The modern English Bulldog is a rather extreme example; their skin folds make them prone to dermatitis; their flat, wide, short-snouted heads make it difficult for them to breathe and regulate their bodily temperature (Boxers have that problem, too); and it is extraordinarily difficult for them to give birth: more than 80 percent of English Bulldog litters are delivered by Caesarian section. There are also additional health concerns related to the limited gene pool; for example, Boxers and Golden Retrievers are at extremely high risk of developing cancer. 

It's worth noting that not all dog breeds are recognized by kennel clubs, but major kennel club recognition can have an effect on genetic diversity within a breed. In a 2003 paper published in the Journal of Heredity, researchers found a much lower correlation between heterozygosity (having different alleles at a particular genetic locus) of a breed with its population size than with the dates the breed was recognized by the AKC registry. Breeds that had been recognized by the AKC more recently had a significantly greater heterozygosity than breeds that had long been on the AKC registry.

Some breed enthusiasts oppose kennel club conformation standards for a reason that is much more pertinent to our discussion here: because it emphasizes morphological traits over behavioral ones. The United States Border Collie Club (USBCC) was founded in 1975 in part to oppose the AKC's use of appearance standards, rather than behavioral standards, for Border Collies. The Border Collie is, after all, a herding dog, and fans of the breed tend to prize it precisely for those qualities that make them good herders, such as stalking, chasing, and grab-biting. A Border Collie, by that rationale, should not be judged primarily for the shape of its body, and certainly not for its willingness to hold its head up in a conformation ring. If you're interested in more on the relationship between the AKC and Border Collies, Donald McCaig documents how the dispute played out in the 1990s in his book The Dog Wars: How the Border Collie Battled the American Kennel Club.

Certain behaviors, such as those related to hunting and other working skills, do correlate strongly with breeds. But does that mean that breeds truly fit their personality stereotypes?

Breeds and their Behaviors

Dogs are particularly interesting subjects for genetic research. The canine genome has been sequenced, and dogs vary wildly in terms of not just morphology, but behavior. Dogs are the most physically varied animal to walk the Earth, and they also have divergent emotional experiences, ones that differ from those of their wolf brethren. A 2004 study published in Molecular Brain Research compared gene expression in the hypothalamus, which controls certain emotional and endocrine responses, in wild canids and dogs, and found that while gene expression was highly conserved in wild canids, it was much more divergent in wolves, suggesting that the behavioral selection that occurred during domestication resulted in rapid changes in brain gene selection.

It seems that every week we are learning something new about the genetics behind dogs' physical appearances, and there are various research projects underway to better understand the genetic links to canine behavior. In 1965, John Paul Scott and John L. Fuller analyzed the genetic component of our dogs' personalities by testing various behavioral traits across breeds for their seminal text Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog, and our understanding of behavioral genetics is growing with time and technology. A 2006 study found a pattern of co-inheritance for sixteen different traits in two breeds of dog. In 2011, Behavioral Genetics published the results of a 50-year study to map the loci of genes for domestication and aggression in foxes. The University of California, San Francisco, has collected DNA and surveys related to thousands of dogs for its Canine Behavioral Genetics Project, and it will be interesting to see what the project's research ultimately yields. But much of our understanding of breeds and behavior comes from decades of human observation.

The American Journal of Human Genetics paper "Canine Behavioral Genetics: Point Out he Phenotypes and Herding up the Genes" provides an excellent round-up of canine behavior as understood in 2008. There are certain behaviors that correlate so closely with breeds, which members of these breeds perform without any sort of encouragement or training, that they must have a genetic component. These traits are most obvious in working breeds. As we mentioned earlier, you want a herding dog that will interact with livestock, stalking it, chasing it, and nipping at its legs. However, if you want a dog that guards livestock, such as a Great Pyrenees, you want a dog that will not stalk or interact with livestock. The Human Genetics paper notes that behaviors like pointing, retrieving, tracking, and drafting likely have strong genetic components. Humans have selected for these behaviors in their hunting and working dogs, and it appears to have worked.

Now, certain physical and behavioral traits may be linked. In the famous "Farm-Fox" experiment launched by geneticist Dmitri Belyaev, the researcher bred silver foxes who were friendly towards people, creating and increasingly reliable stock of tame foxes. As he bred his tame foxes, their appearances changed over the generations; their pricked ears folded over and they became more puppy-like in appearance. Border Collies suffer disproportionately from noise phobia, which may be a byproduct of a desired trait—the one that makes Border Collies able to obey a voice command or whistle given from hundreds of feet away.

However, we must be careful not to assume that, just because a trait may have a genetic component, it exists across members of a breed. After all, we've established that Border Collies raised for the show ring show different behaviors than Border Collies raised for work, and we'll need more research into whether show Border Collies are suppressing a natural urge, or if they differ genetically from their working cousins. And behavioral traits that we may think of as common in certain breeds aren't necessarily universal; Janis Bradley's essay inThe Bark about her lazy greyhound—and whether greyhounds are particularly predisposed toward racing behavior—is particularly enlightening.

How You Affect Your Dog's Personality

Anyone who has owned a dog knows that their personality doesn't spring completely from genetics. Just like humans, dogs are affected by their experiences and their environment—and their personalities can change over time. I strongly suspect that my usually sweet Skoda turns into a bundle of snapping rage at the veterinarian's office because of some painful mange treatments he endured while in foster care. And dogs that have suffered abuse at the hands of humans frequently have deep (but not necessarily indelible) emotional scars.

The idea that dogs' personalities are affected by their experiences is a key component of dog training. Common wisdom holds that the window for ensuring your dog socializes well with other dogs is very narrow; after the first 16 weeks of the puppy's life, the window is supposedly closed. Plus, the current emphasis on positive, reward-based training is based on numerous studies on the effects of positive and negative feedback dog behavior.
There is a particularly fascinating paper published in Behavioural Processes in 2009 that looks at how dogs' personality traits link to non-breed characteristics of dogs as well as the demographics of their owners. The study found that reported calmness correlated with the following factors: whether a dog was neutered or intact, the age of the dog, the age of the owner, the owner's level of education, the age at which the dog was acquired by the owner, how many hours the dog and the owner spent together, and how much training the dog received. This is, of course, just one study, but research is increasingly looking at dogs not just as collections of genes and responses to stimuli, but also as fellow inhabitants in our human environments. We're still learning how dogs develop within human-controlled environments, but it's clear that this is a large part of what makes your dog who he or she is.

What About Mutts?



More than half of all dogs in the United States are mixed breeds, and but even if your mutt has purebred grandparents, you can't necessarily predict its nature. Scott and Fuller studied breed differences in American Cocker Spaniels and Basenjis (as well as other breeds), such as reactivity to a doorbell, problem-solving, and spatial relations, and then crossbred the dogs. They found that the first generation offspring tended to have intermediate performance on behavioral tests relative to their parents, although behavioral patterns grew more complicated in subsequent generations. Nearly three decades later, geneticist Jasper Rine performed a crossbreeding experiment of his own, breeding his Border Collie Gregor (named, of course, for Gregor Mendel) with his Newfoundland Pepper. The first generation of puppies, as with Scott and Fuller's Cocker-Basenji mixes, exhibited a combination of their parents' traits: they loved water like Gregor and hunted down tennis balls with the eagerness of Pepper. But Pepper and Gregor's traits weren't so clearly combined in the next generation of pups, their grandpuppies. These puppies had some of Pepper and Gregor's traits, but the combinations of those traits varied from puppy to puppy. While the behavioral characteristics of Pepper and Gregor's children were predictable to Rine, the characteristics of their grandchildren were not. Just because one of a particular puppy's grandparents was a Newfoundland, that didn't mean the puppy had any interest in swimming.

There's another hazard to assuming a mutt's personality based on its presumed parentage: you might have the parentage wrong. When Scott and Fuller bred their Cocker-Basenji mixes, the second generation of pups showed a great variety in physical appearance and didn't look much at all like the original parents. (Edit: A study released by the National Canine Research Council found that dogs of unknown origin are frequently mis-labeled when it comes to breed.) Appearance, it turns out, isn't a reliable indicator of breed, so if you're using a dog's physical form as clues to its personality, you'll have little luck if that dog is a mystery breed.
Top photo by Found Animals.

READ THIS STORY AND MORE AT......